New research by Kellogg Professor Adam Galinsky shows that clean smells unconsciously promote moral behavior
10/26/2009 - People are unconsciously fairer and more generous when they are in clean-smelling environments, according to a forthcoming study by three academic experts.
The research found a dramatic improvement in ethical behavior with just a few spritzes of citrus-scented Windex.
The piece, which will appear in a forthcoming issue of
Psychological Science, is co-authored by Adam Galinsky, the Morris and Alice Kaplan Professor of Ethics and Decision in Management at the Kellogg School; the lead author is Katie Liljenquist, assistant professor of organizational leadership at BYU’s Marriott School of Management. Co-author Chen-Bo Zhong of the University of Toronto’s Rotman School of Management also contributed to the study.
The researchers see implications for workplaces, retail stores and other organizations that have relied on traditional surveillance and security measures to enforce rules.
“Companies often employ heavy-handed interventions to regulate conduct, but they can be costly or oppressive,” said Liljenquist. “This is a very simple, unobtrusive way to promote ethical behavior.”
Perhaps the findings could be applied at home, too, Liljenquist said: “Could be that getting our kids to clean up their rooms might help them clean up their acts, too.”
The study, titled “The Smell of Virtue,” was unusually simple and conclusive. Participants engaged in several tasks, the only difference being that some worked in unscented rooms, while others worked in rooms freshly spritzed with Windex.
The first experiment evaluated fairness. As a test of whether clean scents would enhance reciprocity, participants played a classic “trust game.” Subjects received $12 of real money (allegedly sent by an anonymous partner in another room). They had to decide how much of it to either keep or return to their partners, who had trusted them to divide it fairly. Subjects in clean-scented rooms were less likely to exploit the trust of their partners, returning a significantly higher share of the money.
- The average amount of cash given back by the people in the “normal” room was $2.81. But the people in the clean-scented room gave back an average of $5.33.
The second experiment evaluated whether clean scents would encourage charitable behavior. Subjects indicated their interest in volunteering with a campus organization for a Habitat for Humanity service project and their interest in donating funds to the cause.
- Participants surveyed in a Windex-ed room were significantly more interested in volunteering (4.21 on a 7-point scale) than those in a normal room (3.29).
- 22 percent of Windex-ed room participants said they’d like to donate money, compared to only 6 percent of those in a normal room.
Follow-up questions confirmed that participants didn’t notice the scent in the room and that their mood at the time of the experiment didn’t affect the outcomes.
“Basically, our study shows that morality and cleanliness can go hand-in-hand,” Galinsky said. “Researchers have known for years that scents play an active role in reviving positive or negative experiences. Now, our research can offer more insight into the links between a person’s charitable actions and their surroundings.”
While this study examined the influence of the physical environment on morality, Zhong and Liljenquist previously published work that demonstrated an intimate link between morality and physical cleanliness. Their 2006 paper in Science reported that transgressions activated a desire to be physically cleansed.
Liljenquist is now researching how perceptions of cleanliness shape our impressions of people and organizations. “The data tell a compelling story about how much we rely upon cleanliness cues to make a wide range of judgments about others,” she said.